Phonology
Phonology is a branch of linguistics
concerned with the systematic organization of sounds in languages. It
has traditionally focused largely on the study of the systems of phonemes
in particular languages
(and therefore used to be also called phonemics, or phonematics),
but it may also cover any linguistic analysis either at a level beneath
the word (including syllable, onset and rime,
articulatory gestures, articulatory
features, mora, etc.) or at all levels of language where sound is considered to be
structured for conveying linguistic meaning. Phonology also includes the
study of equivalent organizational systems in sign
languages.
The
word phonology (as in the phonology
of English) can also refer to the phonological system (sound
system) of a given language. This is one of the fundamental systems which a
language is considered to comprise, like its syntax and its vocabulary.
Phonology
is often distinguished from phonetics. While phonetics concerns the
physical production, acoustic transmission and perception
of the sounds of speech,[1][2]
phonology describes the way sounds function within a given language or across
languages to encode meaning. For many linguists, phonetics belongs to descriptive linguistics, and phonology to theoretical linguistics, although establishing
the phonological system of a language is necessarily an application of
theoretical principles to analysis of phonetic evidence. Note that this
distinction was not always made, particularly before the development of the
modern concept of the phoneme in the mid 20th century. Some subfields of modern
phonology have a crossover with phonetics in descriptive disciplines such as psycholinguistics
and speech perception, resulting in specific areas
like articulatory phonology or laboratory phonology.
Development
of phonology
The
history of phonology may be traced back to the Ashtadhyayi, the Sanskrit grammar
composed by Pāṇini in the 4th century BC. In particular the Shiva Sutras, an
auxiliary text to the Ashtadhyayi, introduces what can be considered a
list of the phonemes of the Sanskrit language, with a notational system for
them that is used throughout the main text, which deals with matters of morphology, syntax and semantics.
The
Polish scholar Jan Baudouin de Courtenay (together with his former student Mikołaj Kruszewski) introduced the concept of the phoneme in 1876,
and his work, though often unacknowledged, is considered to be the starting
point of modern phonology. He also worked on the theory of phonetic
alternations (what is now called allophony and morphophonology), and had a
significant influence on the work of Ferdinand de Saussure.
Nikolai Trubetzkoy, 1920s
An
influential school of phonology in the interwar period was the Prague school. One of its
leading members was Prince Nikolai Trubetzkoy, whose Grundzüge der Phonologie (Principles
of Phonology), published posthumously in 1939, is among the most important
works in the field from this period. Directly influenced by Baudouin de
Courtenay, Trubetzkoy is considered the founder of morphophonology, although
this concept had also been recognized by de Courtenay. Trubetzkoy also
developed the concept of the archiphoneme. Another
important figure in the Prague school was Roman Jakobson, who was
one of the most prominent linguists of the 20th century.
In 1968 Noam Chomsky and Morris Halle published The Sound Pattern of English (SPE), the
basis for generative phonology. In this view, phonological representations are
sequences of segments made up of distinctive features. These features were an expansion of earlier work by
Roman Jakobson, Gunnar Fant, and Morris
Halle. The features describe aspects of articulation and perception, are from a
universally fixed set, and have the binary values + or −. There are at least
two levels of representation: underlying representation and surface phonetic
representation. Ordered phonological rules govern how underlying representation is transformed into the actual
pronunciation (the so-called surface form). An important consequence of the
influence SPE had on phonological theory was the downplaying of the syllable and
the emphasis on segments. Furthermore, the generativists folded morphophonology into
phonology, which both solved and created problems.
Natural
phonology is a theory based on the publications of its proponent David Stampe in 1969 and (more explicitly) in
1979. In this view, phonology is based on a set of universal phonological processes that interact with one another;
which ones are active and which are suppressed is language-specific. Rather
than acting on segments, phonological processes act on distinctive features within prosodic groups. Prosodic groups can be as
small as a part of a syllable or as large as an entire utterance. Phonological
processes are unordered with respect to each other and apply simultaneously
(though the output of one process may be the input to another). The second most
prominent natural phonologist is Patricia Donegan (Stampe's wife); there are
many natural phonologists in Europe, and a few in the U.S., such as Geoffrey
Nathan. The principles of natural phonology were extended to morphology by Wolfgang U. Dressler, who founded natural morphology.
In
1976 John Goldsmith introduced autosegmental phonology. Phonological phenomena are no
longer seen as operating on one linear sequence of segments, called
phonemes or feature combinations, but rather as involving some parallel
sequences of features which reside on multiple tiers. Autosegmental
phonology later evolved into feature geometry, which
became the standard theory of representation for theories of the organization
of phonology as different as lexical phonology and optimality theory.
Government phonology, which originated in the early 1980s as an attempt to
unify theoretical notions of syntactic and phonological structures, is based on
the notion that all languages necessarily follow a small set of principles and vary
according to their selection of certain binary parameters. That is,
all languages' phonological structures are essentially the same, but there is
restricted variation that accounts for differences in surface realizations.
Principles are held to be inviolable, though parameters may sometimes come into
conflict. Prominent figures in this field include Jonathan Kaye, Jean Lowenstamm, Jean-Roger Vergnaud, Monik
Charette, and John Harris.
In a
course at the LSA summer institute in 1991, Alan Prince and Paul Smolensky developed optimality theory—an overall
architecture for phonology according to which languages choose a pronunciation
of a word that best satisfies a list of constraints ordered by importance; a
lower-ranked constraint can be violated when the violation is necessary in
order to obey a higher-ranked constraint.
The
approach was soon extended to morphology by John McCarthy and Alan Prince, and has
become a dominant trend in phonology. The appeal to phonetic grounding of
constraints and representational elements (e.g. features) in various approaches
has been criticized by proponents of 'substance-free phonology', especially Mark Hale and Charles Reiss.
Broadly speaking, government phonology (or its
descendant, strict-CV phonology) has a greater following in the United Kingdom,
whereas optimality theory is predominant in the United States.
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar